Skip to main content

If we want to persuade anti-vaxxers, aggression is not the way

In recent days, many European political leaders (such as the Italian and French premiers) seem to have identified the public enemy number one in the so-called no-vaxxers.

Thus the Italian Prime Minister Mario Draghi: “Hospitals are suffering from the impact the virus has on the unvaccinated population. Many of the problems we have now are due to the unvaccinated population that occupies two thirds of the beds in intensive care”.

So instead the French Prime Minister Emmanuel Macron: “With the unvaccinated, I really want to get on their nerves. And so we will continue to do that, until the end. This is the strategy”.

I got vaccinated at the earliest opportunity. I have already received my third dose. I firmly believe that vaccines are a privilege (a privilege largely inaccessible in many other countries) and are saving us from other painful lockdowns and the collapse of our economy.

Definitely, inviting all citizens to get vaccinated is desirable.

However, I don't think the frontal attack against no-vaxxers is a good strategy.

Nor is isolating a minority in our society - even if we consider it a minority of naive, poorly informed people.

Nor fueling further social conflicts and exacerbate spirits already shaken by two years of pandemic.

This strategy risks to elicit a reaction opposite to what we want.

It is not the first time a minority (a small but noisy minority - at least online) openly disputes the validity and safety of vaccines.

Indeed, the vaccine hesitancy has an ancient history, with different shades ranging from a just whispered skepticism to the rabid virulence of novax movements.

Maybe you remember that even a few years ago, when Covid could exist just in some dystopias, there were a lot of concerned warnings on web and social media about an unlikely link between vaccines and autism.

Apparently, this fake news originated by a research of almost 80 years ago, spoiled by economic interests and already retracted at the time.

Yet, as the Harvard Business Review noted in 2015, a third (!) of American parents still believed that old research and about a quarter deemed celebrities were a credible source on vaccines.

Sadly, this odd and anti-historical movement of opinion achieved a concrete, dramatic result: the new and unexpected spread of measles in states where it had disappeared for years.

At the time, as today with vaccines against coronavirus, a huge body of empirical evidence was not enough to change the mind of many people.

A lot of them have paid for their stubbornness with pain and death.

What motivates this irrational and self-defeating obstinacy?

One of the reasons is that often, as human beings, we only accept information that confirms and reinforces our previous beliefs, rejecting instead those that questions our inner system of principles and values (the so-called confirmation bias).

And, when factual evidence clearly dismantles our ideas, sometimes a paradoxical effect is produced: instead of abandoning or updating our ideas, we keep and defend them with even greater determination (the so-called backfire effect).

The latter phenomenon was described by a 2010 study, where evidence that at the time of Bush Iraq did not possess mass destruction weapons did not affect the beliefs of the war supporters, but rather ended up strengthening them (however, there are different views on the backfire effect).

The paradox is only apparent: sometimes, we just do not want our beliefs - the pillars of our identity - to collapse like a house of cards.

Those believing vaccines are dangerous and vaccination obligation is unfair will probably remain tied to their vision in spite of empirical evidence, because the defense of their values and identity will prevail over the factual reality.

What to do then?

Surely mass media should not give too much space to positions which are largely a minority not only in the scientific community, but also in the public opinion.

Democracy does not mean that everyone can talk about everything!

Aside from this, some research suggests that persuading others of our reasons and correcting their misperceptions is a demanding but possible goal.

The key is to show respect for the values and worldviews of our conversation partners: corrective information is more likely to be accepted if our arguments are not perceived as a threat to their identity and an undue invasion of their belief system.

One of the rhetorical tactics to overcome these defensive mechanisms may be to recognize their reasons on other topics, strengthening their self-esteem and making them more prone to second thoughts on the controversial topic we are discussing.

However, the various surveys provide partially conflicting conclusions on the incidence of self-affirmation (that is our ability to respond and adapt to information and experiences that question our idea of ourselves).

Last but not least, research also suggests that corrective information is more effective in a graphical form than in an only textual one.

Infographics, bar charts and pie charts are valuable allies for an authoritative information.

But we insiders already knew that, right?

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Brands need a strong identity, not to cling to someone else's. The Balocco and Ferragni affair

The fact : the Italian Competition Authority fined the companies of the famous influencer Chiara Ferragni , along with the confectionery company Balocco , with a total penalty of €1,075,000 for unfair commercial practices related to the Pink Christmas pandoro. The fine for Balocco is €420,000. The Antitrust claims that the companies led consumers to believe that by purchasing the pandoro, they were contributing to a donation to the Regina Margherita Hospital in Turin , when in reality, the €50,000 donation had already been made by Balocco months earlier. Companies associated with Chiara Ferragni collected over one million euros from the initiative. How did they get to this point? First mistake: confusion  What was the goal of the marketing campaign ?  To improve the company's ethical reputation or to reach the audience that adores Chiara Ferragni?  They are two different levels: the first is moral, the second is hedonistic.  Why muddy the waters? If the goal was to improve the co

The Kit Kat & Twix case. Does kindness really pay off for brands?

  Sometimes, when we think about marketing, an aggressive and unforgiving environment comes to our mind. After all, market is competition , right? However, many people value integrity and fair play . And they wish to reward brands that embrace these values. Is flattery more beneficial to the giver or the recipient? In 2021, in a series of 12 experiments led by Duke University, two groups of consumers were shown two fake tweets (when we could still call them tweets...) by Kit Kat (snacks that I suppose needs no introduction): First tweet - Kit Kat praises Twix : @twix, Competitor or not, congrats on your 54 years in business! Even we can admit - Twix are delicious Second tweet - Kit Kat praises itself :  Start your day off with a tasty treat! 11 days later, the percentage of those who bought a Kit Kat : was 31.95% among those who had seen the first tweet - the competitor's praise was 23.77% among those who had seen the second tweet - the trivial self-praise And the delicious Twix

Too much jam can be confusing: the Paradox of Choice

December : a time of carefree afternoons spent shopping with your loved ones.  As you stroll through the crowded, festive streets illuminated with a thousand colors, your attention as food enthusiasts is caught by two stands on either side of the street.  Both sell artisanal jams: on one counter there are 24 variants; on the other counter there are only 6 variants.  Which of the two stands is more likely to pique your interest?  Which of the two stands is more likely to turn you into their customers?  The answers to these two questions may not coincide .  Too much choice leads to no choice  In 2000, psychologists Sheena Iyengar and Mark Lepper conducted an experiment to check the correlation between the number of alternatives available and the customer conversion rate .  The experiment didn't take place in Christmas markets but in Menlo Park, California, in an upscale supermarket, where potential customers were lured with a $1 discount voucher.  The results seem counterintuitive: